Ready?
One day, after Little Red Riding hood woke up, mother called her into the kitchen and handed her a basket of cakes and pastries. “Take these to grandmother. She's sick, and perhaps these cakes will make her feel better.”
If you have been reading for years, you probably found it difficult, if not impossible, to keep yourself from reading. You may have kept yourself from understanding the sentence, but odds are, you probably understood some of the words. When I try to do it, I have to blur my vision because if any letters come into focus, I automatically decode their meaning. For most adults in literate countries, reading is so well practiced that it’s reflexive. If the words are there, it's impossible not to read.
Now think about this for a minute. Here we have a completely artificial task. It’s not part of our biology – humans aren’t born with an innate reading reflex. If you raise a child on a desert island, he'll learn to eat, walk, and sleep, but odds are he won't spontaneously pick up a stick and start writing. For most of human history, written language didn't even exist. Reading as a cultural invention has only been around for a few thousand years, a snap of a finger in evolutionary terms. We have not, and will not within any of our lifetimes, evolve a genetic program for reading. Yet our brains are so adept at this skill that it becomes as reflexive as seeing itself.
That we are able to become so skilled at reading is a testament to the flexibility and plasticity of our brains. Of course, we don't start from scratch. We already have fine tuned machinery for similar tasks – we’re very good at seeing, and the trick is just to retune that machinery to the demands of reading. These demands are multiple. On one hand there’s the mapping from sound to symbol, and getting our visual and language systems to work together. But even on a basic visual level, we have to somewhat reprogram our visual systems.
For example, imagine a horse. Now in your head, imagine that the horse is standing in front of a mirror, and you're looking at its reflection. What does it look like now? Of course, it still looks like a horse. Mirror invariance, the idea that something flipped sideways is still the same object, is a core property of our visual systems, and for good reason. Imagine how confusing it would be if every time we saw something from the opposite direction, it became something different. But now, imagine the lowercase letter b. What's the mirror image of b? Now it's a completely different letter: d. In most writing systems, left-right orientation matters. When a character is flipped, we see it as something else.
So what happens when you take a brain used to making mirror generalizations and teach it to read? Any parents of school-age children could tell you. When you're first learning, you make lots of mistakes. Mirror reversal is overwhelmingly common in beginning writers, from the occasional flipped letter to whole words written as a mirror image. Kids do this spontaneously. They never actually see flipped letters in the world around them. It's as if their brains are too powerful for the task. They generalize letters to other orientations because that's what they've done with every single other object they've seen. You have to learn to recognize a letter only in a certain orientation .
With practice however, we do retrain our brains to read, and as you saw when we did the letter counting exercise, we become quite good at it. With such an ingrained skill, it makes you wonder if you can see its footprints in the brain. Does the brain of a reader look different from that of a nonreader?
To answer this question, we must first step back and see how our brain’s visual regions are organized. Much of the research in this area has been conducted by Nancy Kanwisher, a psychologist at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Her primary tool is fMRI, an imaging technology that allows neuroscientists to measure blood flow in the brain as people perform tasks. Since blood flow is tied to brain activity, fMRI allows us to see the patches of brain involved in different tasks. Kanwisher’s experiments often involve participants looking at different categories of images. By looking for areas that respond more to one category than another, she can find brain regions specialized in dealing with a certain image type. Using this technique, Kanwisher has found many specialized regions: a face area, a place area, and even an area that responds preferentially to body parts. It makes sense that these areas would exist. As a species, our survival has long depended on our ability to recognize and process faces, places, and body parts, so it's not surprising that we would have developed brain regions specialized for those things.
But what about words? Would they have their own region? A few years ago, Chris Baker, a researcher in Kanwisher’s lab, conducted the same type of experiment to look for a word region. Baker scanned native English speakers while they looked at different types of images, including words, line drawings, and Hebrew letters. They found that most participants did indeed have a brain region that responded more to words than objects.
This is rather remarkable, that the brain would develop a specialized area for an artificial category of images. But to fully interpret the results, we need more proof that this region developed as a result of learning to read. It could be that the region is there simply by coincidence, that the area is responding to some visual characteristic of print words regardless of whether a person is literate or not. To follow up, Baker tested monolingual English readers and bilingual English/Hebrew readers on the same experiment. If reading experience does alter the brain, you would expect English readers and English/Hebrew readers to have different brain responses to Hebrew. And this is indeed what Baker found. The bilingual readers had high activation for both Hebrew and English in their word region, while monolingual English readers only had high activation for English. Experience with a written language does indeed shape the brain’s response to that language.
The above passage is an excerpt from my essay From Words to Brain, which follows the reading process from the moment a reader sees words on a page, through understanding the story and ultimately extracting meaning.
Buy the essay from:
Baker CI, Liu J, Wald LL, Kwong KK, Benner T, & Kanwisher N (2007). Visual word processing and experiential origins of functional selectivity in human extrastriate cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104 (21), 9087-92 PMID: 17502592
I tried the experiment but couldn't stop "reading" some of the words.
ReplyDeleteThe brain is such a fascinating subject!
Very interesting! There's a lot of dyslexia in my family and some of my siblings, myself included when I am tired, still reverse letters (writing a d when b is meant etc.)
ReplyDeleteI was going to say, this is from your work. I'm finding the topic so fascinating to read. Can't wait to do a review of this on my blog.
ReplyDeleteCD
Marisa -- Have you heard of the stroop task? that's another great demo of how automatic reading is
ReplyDeleteCarolyn -- It's awesome that you're writing books with family history of dyslexia. That's amazing.
Clarissa -- Sharp eye :-) Thanks again for reviewing.
Livia, I have posted my comment on Amazon.com and Amazon.co.uk. I wanted to post it here too, but my paste pastes something else. Apologies! I can't copy it again, because I closed the window and Amazon are holding it for vetting before they publish it.
ReplyDeleteAnyway, it is a very favourable review, but it was hard to keep my promise to not (directly) criticize Kindle. Kindle has removed the superscript from the citation numbers and shown them in a miniscule font so they are much harder to see than the "a-s" in your exercise. And, worse, Kindle has hidden the citations right at the back of the essay behind trailers for other essays and books.
*Tip for readers of this blog*:
"From words to brain" is well worth owning. Get the PDF version from Smashwords, which is much easier to read - and, you can print it out.
I found this absolutely fascinating (I really struggled not to 'read' the sentences and ended up having to go from the end and work letter by letter back to the start... and even then I read some of the words!)
ReplyDeleteThere is also an exhibit at the British Library about language (more than reading) and dialect, which looks to be interesting: it is looking at how language is evolving.
Thanks :)
twaza -- Thank you so much for your kind review! I passed along your footnote comment to my publisher, and they're trying to see if they can fix it. Amazon is tricky because they are by far the most influential retailer and have the best recommendation engines. If a book sells well on Amazon, it tends to snowball because Amazon starts recommending it to new customers in a very intelligent way. Unfortunately, Amazon chooses to only sell their kindle format, and you're absolutely right that it's not the best format for people who do not use ereading devices. I'd say that if you are used to reading paper books, you should go for the PDF that can print out. If you are accustomed to reading on a Kindle, iPad, or smartphone, then the Kindle format has more flexibility in terms of layout and font resizeability.
ReplyDeleteGrillyfish -- Haha, you read it backwards! That's awesome.
What would be really interesting would be to see what this implies about dyslexia and other reading disorders, if anything.
ReplyDeleteThis is an absolutely fascinating concept. I could not stop myself from reading the words.
ReplyDeleteNow, since I'm not a brain scientist, this might be a dumb question, but I'll ask anyway.
This region in the brain that activates when it sees words, is it a 'new' region that the brain developed? Or is the brain utilising a region that was already there, but not used?
alleyjack - it's still an open question. The field has moved away from the idea that dyslexia is "seeing things backwards." It seems that all children do it, and people notice it more when the child is dyslexic. But then, I still hear a lot of personal reports from people with dyslexia about flipping letters, so I'm not sure how to resolve that.
ReplyDeleteJake -- that's actually a really good question -- one that is still being pursued. Recent research suggests that the region takes over part of the face processing region. Literate people have smaller face processing regions than illiterate people. That's not the whole story though. More research is needed.
I found it impossible not to read the words. The exercise reminded me of the one where all the words are misspelt, but contain the right letters. Somehow our brain unscrambles them.
ReplyDeleteYour blog is fascinating, Livia. I've linked to you on my own blog.